@article{RIUMI_2014_1_7_3_397_0, author = {Valeria Giardino}, title = {Matematica e cognizione}, journal = {La Matematica nella Societ\`a e nella Cultura. Rivista dell'Unione Matematica Italiana}, volume = {7}, year = {2014}, pages = {397-415}, zbl = {1391.00028}, language = {it}, url = {http://dml.mathdoc.fr/item/RIUMI_2014_1_7_3_397_0} }
Giardino, Valeria. Matematica e cognizione. La Matematica nella Società e nella Cultura. Rivista dell'Unione Matematica Italiana, Tome 7 (2014) pp. 397-415. http://gdmltest.u-ga.fr/item/RIUMI_2014_1_7_3_397_0/
[1]
(a cura di) (2005), Handbook of mathematical cognition, Psychology Press, New York.[2] The cognitive basis of mathematical knowledge', in , e (a cura di), Mathematical Knowledge, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 73-83. | MR 2450933 | Zbl 1328.97011
e (2007), `[3] Where our number concepts come from', Journal of Philosophy, vol. 106, pp. 220-54.
(2009), `[4]
(1993), Les enjeux du mobile. Mathématique, physique, philosophie, Collection des Travayx, Edition du Seuil, Paris.[5] A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation', Cognition, vol. 23, pp. 149-78.
(1986), `[6] The numerical attribute of stimuli', in , e (a cura di), Animal Cognition, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (NJ), pp. 445-64.
e (1984), `[7] | MR 2985901 | Zbl 1255.00003
(2012), An Introduction to the philosophy of mathematics, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.[8] Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group', Science, vol. 311, pp. 381-84. | MR 2193091 | Zbl 1226.01001
, , e (2006), `[9] Mathematical symbols as epistemic actions', Synthese, vol. 190, pp. 3-19. | Zbl 1284.00039
e (2013), `[10] Forms and roles of diagrams in knot theory', Erkenntnis, vol. 79, pp. 829-42. | MR 3260948 | Zbl 1304.57013
e (2014), `[11] An inquiry into the practice of proving in low-dimensional topology', in , e (a cura di), From Logic to Practice: Italian Studies in the Philosophy of Mathematics (Boston Studies in the Philosophy and History of Science, Vol. 308), Springer, Berlin, pp. 315-36. | MR 3329904
e (2015), `[12] The representations underlying infants' choice of more: object files versus analog magnitudes', Psychological Science, vol. 13, pp. 150-56.
, e (2002), `[13]
(1983), The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).[14] | MR 2345085 | Zbl 1175.00023
(2007), Visual thinking in mathematics, Oxford University Press, Oxford.[15] I fondamenti cognitivi della matematica umana: dove cercarli?', in , e (a cura di), Animali Razionali. Studi sui confini e sulle possibilità della razionalità, numero speciale di Isonomia - Epistemologica.
(di prossima pubblicazione), `[16]
(2003), Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think, Belknap Press, Cambridge (MA).[17] Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia', Science, vol. 306, pp. 496-99.
(2004), `[18] | MR 2343928 | Zbl 1209.00016
(2007), Representation and productive ambiguity in mathematics and the sciences, Oxford University Press, Oxford.[19] A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children', Nature, vol. 370, pp. 57-59.
e (1994), `[20] Modularity and development: The case of spatial reorientation', Cognition, vol. 61, pp. 195-232.
e (1996), `[21] Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: The case of two spatial memory tasks', Cognition, vol. 79, pp. 263-99.
, e (2001), `[22] | MR 2145193 | Zbl 1081.00005
(a cura di) (2006), 18 Unconventional Essays on the Nature of Mathematics, Springer, Berlin.[23] Distributed cognition', in e (a cura di), The International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences, Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 2068-72.
(2001), `[24] A credo of sorts', in e (a cura di), Truth in mathematics, Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 203-14. | MR 1688343 | Zbl 1041.57500
(1998), `[25] Core systems in human cognition', Progress in Brain Research, vol. 164, pp. 257-64.
e (2007), `[26] The ability of birds to count', Bulletin of Animal Behaviour, vol. 9, pp. 41-45.
(1951), `[27]
(1962), The structure of scientific revolutions, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.[28]
e (1980), Metaphors we live by, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.[29] | MR 1794854
e (2000), Where mathematics come from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being, Basic Books, New York.[30] Seeing how it goes: Paper-and-pencil reasoning in mathematical practice', Philosophia Mathematica, vol. 20, pp. 58-85. | MR 2889175 | Zbl 1266.97007
(2012), `[31] Use of numbers by a chimpanzee', Nature, vol. 315, pp. 57-59.
(1985), `[32]
(2008), Creating scientific concepts, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA).[33] | MR 1683176 | Zbl 1025.01002
(1999), The shaping of deduction in Greek mathematics: A study of cognitive history, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.[34] Whorf versus Socrates, round 10', Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol. 10, pp. 394-96.
e (2006), `[35]
(1948/1960), The child's conception of geometry, Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.[36]
(1952), The child's conception of number, Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.[37] Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group', Science, vol. 306, pp. 499-503.
, , e (2004), `[38] Is it only humans that count from left to right?', Biology Letters, vol. 6, pp. 290-92.
, , , e (2010), `[39] A constructive response to where mathematics comes from', Educational Studies in Mathematics, vol. 52, pp. 79-91.
e (2003), `[40] Mathematical practice and conceptual metaphors: On cognitive studies of historical developments in mathematics', Topics in Cognitive Science, vol. 5, pp. 283-98.
(2013), `[41] Drawing space: mathematicians' kinetic conceptions of eigenvectors', Educational Studies in Mathematics, vol. 74, pp. 223-40.
e (2010), `[42] Modularity as a fish views it: Conjoining geometric and nongeometric information for spatial reorientation', Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, vol. 29, pp. 199-210.
, e (2003), `[43] Learning of geometry and features in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris)', Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol. 3, pp. 312-18.
, , (2013), `[44] On proof and progress in mathematics', Bullettin of the American Mathematical Society, vol. 30, pp. 161-77. | MR 1249357
(1994), `[45] What representations might underlie infant numerical knowledge', Cognitive Development, vol. 14, pp. 1-36.
, , e (1999), `[46] Geometric modules in animal's spatial representation: A test with chicks', Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol. 104, pp. 248-54.
, e (1990), `[47] The unreasonable effectiveness of mathematics in the natural sciences', Communications on Pure and Applied Mathematics, vol. 13, pp. 1-14. | MR 824292 | Zbl 0102.00703
(1960), `[48] Addition and subtraction by human infants', Nature, vol. 358, pp. 749- 50.
(1992), `